https://youtubetranscript.com/?v=PY4sShDt9to

Music Dr. Richard Heyer studies the neural basis of human intelligence and cognition. He works with neuroimaging technologies to study individual differences in mental ability. He received his PhD in psychology from the Johns Hopkins University in 1975 and has since held appointments in the intramural research program at the National Institute of Mental Health and the medical school at Brown University and the University of California at Irvine. Irvine, he has served on the editorial board of three journals Neuro Image, Intelligence and Psychiatry Research. He also served as guest editor for a special issue on brain imaging research for the journal Intelligence. He provides neuroscience consultation to university research groups, corporations, foundations, and educational and legal professionals. He’s a popular lecturer and has appeared in numerous media outlets. In 2012, his research was featured on Nova Science Now and he received the Distinguished Contributor Award for the International Society for Intelligence Research. In 2013, the teaching company invited Richard to create an 18-lecture course called The Intelligent Brain. So welcome to Dr. Richard Heyer today and we’re going to talk about the most controversy of all topics, I would say, in social science. Strangely enough, intelligence. So maybe we could start with a little bit of historical information. I would like to know how you got interested in IQ research or intelligence research, let’s say. And so let’s start with that and then we can start diving into the nitty gritty. Well really, it started in graduate school at Hopkins when I really became most interested in personality research. And I started out studying individual differences in personality. But just by happenstance, the year I started graduate school in 1971 was the year one of the professors there, Julian Stanley, was starting the study of mathematically precocious youth. And I was one of the proctors at the very first talent search for mathematically precocious kids. And I wrote my first couple of papers as book chapters in books that Stanley was editing about this project. And I saw these kids aged 10, 11, 12, who were scoring higher on SAT math than Hopkins freshmen. And the question was, you know, how does this happen? Where does this come from? So that was kind of my earliest interest. And in graduate school, although I really completed my dissertation on personality, I took my first job at the National Institute of Mental Health in the intramural research program in the laboratory of psychology and psychopathology, which at the time, the lab director was David Rosenthal, who had just finished the Denmark adoption studies of schizophrenia. Right, right. And here is where I learned about genetics. My office was next door to a fellow named Monty Buxbaum, who was doing both potential search. Yeah. I’m very interested in that. And so my early interest in individual differences slowly morphed into an interest in individual differences in intelligence. And at NIMH, they were just going through a transition from kind of a psychoanalytic orientation to a neuroscience orientation. And I was kind of caught up in that. And so that’s the origin of my interest in the brain and in technologies to make brain measurements and relate that to individual differences. OK, right. OK, now you just wrote a book, too, in the neuroscience of intelligence, came to the University Press. And so when did that come out? That came out really just about six months ago. So it came out, I think, in December of 2016. But they tell me for publishing reasons, they call it a 2017 publication. I see. Well, part of the reason I was so excited to talk to you is that I’ve done a very large amount of research, especially not so much practical lab research, but investigation into the structure of intelligence and into its measurement. We designed back in 93 with a student of mine, Daniel Higgins. I think what was probably the first online battery purporting to measure the cognitive abilities associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right? So hypothetically, the highest order cognitive functions in the brain. And we found much to our chagrin, I would say. And this was a very painful discovery that a lot of what we had been thinking about as potentially separable neuropsychological functions were pretty easily collapsible into good old general intelligence, you know, that killer central factor that seems to unite cognitive abilities. And so it was quite a shock, especially because the neuropsychologists of the time, and they still do this, are as assiduous investigators of the psychometric intelligence literature as they should be and tend to underestimate the central power of that initial factor. So anyways, I’m really interested into intelligence research, partly from a practical perspective too, because the industrial organizational psychology literature is crystal clear. For complex jobs, the best predictor of long-term success is intelligence. And it’s a predictor that’s probably, say, imagine you give it an R of 0.4 to 0.5, which is pretty decent. So let’s say 25% of the variance. The next best predictor is conscientiousness, and it’s pushing its limit at mopping up 10% of the variability in long-term performance. So IQ, it’s a killer, man. And I make sure my students assess intelligence with everything they do, and it always ends up being a major predictor of things that you wouldn’t even expect, like disgust sensitivity, for example. So your disgust sensitivity is higher if you have a lower IQ. Yeah. The G factor is powerful. You just said something, though, that I want to just make a distinction about. You were talking about the G factor, and then you kind of called it IQ. This is very common in everyday language to talk about intelligence, IQ, and what we call the G factor as one thing, and it really isn’t. So an IQ score is a good estimate of the G factor. Right. It also includes other aspects of intelligence. And intelligence itself, although it’s a broad term, is only part of the universe of mental abilities. So if you’re very good at, say, calculating on what day January 5th was in the year 1520, that’s a mental ability that some people have. Doesn’t mean you’re smart. Right, yes. You see that with autistic savants often, that they’re set out often. But sufficiently often, they have these amazing calculation abilities, for example, that don’t seem to be manifest in a spectacularly high overall intelligence. So what do you want to tell us about? You want to start with the book and walk us through it? Well, you know, that’s interesting because the book is kind of a culmination of things I’ve learned mostly from my neuroimaging work on intelligence. And it kind of came as a surprise. It’s the first book I’ve ever written. I’m now retired, actually, from academia. Never wrote a book while I was in academia. I was writing journal papers. But Cambridge University Press called, and they have this series of fundamentals of neuroscience. And they wanted to include intelligence. And I regarded that as a major step because intelligence research really has been relegated almost to the peripheral of mainstream psychology. Yeah. Yeah, politically suspect, to say the least. Yeah. And, you know, the switch happened overnight from being in the mainstream to being really peripheral. In about 1969, before 1969, almost everyone who was interested in education was concerned about the achievement gaps. Right. And they felt universally that once you equalized educational opportunities, those achievement gaps would disappear. Yeah, that was the head start. The head start point. Yeah, even before head start, head start came to head start. But even before head start, there were all these demonstration projects. There was one, the Miracle in Milwaukee. And there were all these things that showed that if you really intervened in early childhood education, which at that time was called compensatory education, the early childhood education term came much later. But this idea of compensatory education really took off. And then in 1969, the Harvard Educational Review asked one of the foremost educational psychologists, Arthur Jensen, to write a review of the progress. And this article in 1969 has become infamous. The opening sentence was essentially, we’ve tried compensatory education and it has failed. Yeah. And then he had 100 pages of detailed statistical analysis of why there were no, you couldn’t demonstrate an increase in IQ score in any of these programs. Now head start had just begun, so head start wasn’t included. Yeah, but I reviewed the literature on head start too extensively. Basically what happened was that, so that was for those of the viewers who don’t know, head start was a nationwide attempt to add additional education to the lives of disadvantaged kids, especially at the preschool level. And basically what happened was that they actually did show improvements in academic achievement initially. So in grade one and grade two, they were performing above their peers, but then the difference in improvement, the difference in performance started to decrease. And then by about the grade five or grade six, the differences had disappeared completely. So there was no evidence whatsoever of that, either of a stable one time long term gain in cognitive ability, or what people were really hoping was that if you intervened early enough, you’d get something that would sort of turn into a positive feedback loop. And the gains would actually advance across time. And what ended up happening with the head start research basically was the conclusion that it produced no cognitive improvements whatsoever, although more kids who went through head start graduated from high school, fewer of them were delinquent, more of them, fewer of them became pregnant in teenage, in the teenage years, and more of them went to colleges. But that seemed to be because they were better socialized, not because they were in any way had been made smarter. So that was a really tremendous disappointment because it was a bipartisan attempt to come to grips with the fundamental issues that sort of bedeviled structural poverty in the United States. No one was happy about that outcome, I can tell you. Well, not only that, but when Jensen published his article, he also said that since IQ increases seem not to be coming from these intense environmental interventions, we should consider the possibility that these differences have a genetic component. And that really began the incendiary descent of intelligence research to the periphery. The reaction against that was universal because it implied a genetic inferiority if you didn’t have genes for IQ. It’s also something that’s universally hated on both sides of the political spectrum because on the liberal end, the idea fundamentally is that everybody’s the same and that if you distribute educational resources properly, then everyone can succeed. And so that didn’t work out so well for the liberals. And then on the conservative side, the idea is, well, if you could just get off your lazy asses and get a job, there is a job for you out there. And the truth of the matter is, you know, you can tell me what you think about this, but this was a statistic that just absolutely shocked and staggered me when I went through the intelligence literature. So, you know, it is illegal in the United States to induct anybody who has an IQ of less than 83. And the reason for that is, you know, that the American armed forces have been conducting intelligence research for like more than 100 years. And that was partly because they needed a way of sorting people rapidly during times of military expansion during wartime. But it was also because IQ tests, especially in the early part of the 20th century, were used to identify, let’s say, the deserving poor who could really benefit from additional educational attainment and advancement. And the military was hoping to identify people from lower class strata that could be streamed into, say, officer training programs and so forth, or even skills training programs, to move people from the underclass into at least the working class and maybe above. So they had a bloody stake in this, man. They wanted to find people. They wanted to sort them properly. And they wanted to do social good when they weren’t just trying to win a war, let’s say, which often also is a social good. But what happened was that by, I don’t remember when this legislation was introduced, but it was in the later part of the 20th century. But their basic finding was that by, say, the 1980s, they had determined that if you had an IQ of less than 83, there was not a damn thing that the army could do, the armed forces could do, to transform you into someone who could do something that was more productive than non-productive. And the terrible thing about that is that it’s about 10% of the population. And so, you look at a statistic like that and you think, oh my god, you’ve got this enterprise, this massive enterprise that’s chronically hungry for people. It’s right, they’re always looking for people. They’re really oriented towards taking people from the underclass and lower working class and pushing them up the societal strata. And during wartime, they’re actually desperate to bring in recruits, period. And their conclusion is that 10% of the population can’t be trained to do anything sufficiently useful to make them militarily operable. It’s just, I just read that, my jaw just dropped. Well, yeah, you know, in the United States, we have about 330 million people. And because of the distribution, the relatively normal distribution of IQ scores, about 16% have IQs of 85 or less. Right, right. Which means they’re not going to graduate school. No, it means that from what I’ve read practically, it means the Wunderlust Company has actually done a really, they have a nice IQ test from the commercial perspective. You know, it’s actually psychometrically valid. And they’ve linked IQ levels to job, specifically to job categories, you know. Yes, I know. And what I was going to say is they’re not only not going to graduate school, they’re not going to find a stable job that pays a livable wage. Yeah. Especially even given that so many of the service jobs now require a fair high degree of computational savvy. Or, not computational, but ability to interact with complex computational technology. Even the typical till at a checkout market, or the till at a McDonald’s, because McDonald’s is actually very complicated, is often far beyond the ability of people who are on the low end of the intelligence distribution. And they claimed, I think it was Wunderlich, although it might have been Hunt. What’s his name? IQ researcher. Is it Earl Hunt, I think? Possibly? He claimed that if you have an IQ of below 90, that it’s difficult for you to read well enough to translate what you’re reading into action. So you can’t actually read instructions and follow them. You don’t have that level of literacy. That’s correct. So I was going to say that in the United States, this bottom 16% translates into 51 million people. Including 13 million children who are in school. This is a very difficult problem. Now I knew Earl Hunt. He passed away last year. I knew him pretty well. He also would say that there is this cognitive segregation in society. This is a point that Charles Murray makes as well. And Earl would often ask, when’s the last time you had someone over for dinner who wasn’t a college grad? Well that was something that Murray and Heernstein wrote about in their book, The Bell Curve, which really struck me. Because I read that book twice, unlike most of the people who criticized it. And one of the things that they pointed out in there was, look, the typical educated person thinks that someone isn’t very bright if they have an IQ of 115. So we’re talking about graduate level and PhD level research institutions. Because 115, there’s as many people at 115 above as there are at 85 and below. So it’s a minority of the population. And that’s the top 15%. And that’s the dollar undergraduate. See, I’m a clinical psychologist and I’ve dealt with people who had ranges in the low 80s. And tried to find them jobs and tried to train them. And I have some real knowledge about the stunning gap between people at the low end of the IQ distribution and the high end. And it’s no bloody wonder people hate IQ research and intelligence research because it reveals a set of seriously dismal facts about the incredible range of ability among human beings. Well, yes, this is true. And moreover, I would add to this that people in universities, professors and graduate students, have a hard time understanding what everyday life is like. If you have an IQ of 85 and you’re making your way, you’re living independently, you’re making your way in the world. But it is a challenge. It is a real challenge. It just barely begins to describe it. I had a client who probably had an IQ of under 80, the non-verbal portion of it anyways. He was indistinguishable in physical appearance from, let’s say, I hate to use the frame normal person, but there’s nothing that marked him out about particularly intellectually impaired. And I tried at one point, this was so telling to me, I got him a volunteer job, which by the way is very difficult. It’s harder to get a volunteer job than a real job because you have to do police screening and all sorts of things and the selection process is just as extreme. But I eventually ended up getting him a job at a bike store, bike slash bookstore. But that place couldn’t hold him once the subsidy program had expired. And then I got him a job at a charity and his job was to fold letters into three so that they could be put into envelopes. Well, that sounds easy, except that he also had a bit of a motor tremor. And you know, it took me about 30 hours to train him to fold up a piece of paper with sufficient precision so that it could be put in an envelope rapidly. So that the envelope wasn’t so mangled that it would get stuck in the automatic sorting machine. And you know, there was high performance demands on him too. He had to whip through those letters pretty quickly. And then sometimes the letters would have a photograph appended to them that was stapled on. And they weren’t always stapled on in the same place. So then he had to calculate how to fold the paper over the photograph without bending the photograph in precise thirds so that it would still fit in the envelope. And then he had to separate the French letters from the English letters and associate them with the proper envelopes. And like that level of complexity just did him in. Let me say two things about this. One is, I hope, common sense. And the other is pretty provocative. The common sense thing is we have to be very careful when we have these discussions not to devalue the human dignity of people who aren’t in the upper end of the distribution. And if there’s one criticism that I think is fair is sometimes in these conversations it sounds like we’re devaluing people at the lower end of the distribution. And we have to be very careful that we don’t do that. Human life has dignity and IQ is not the most important thing that defines human beings. Yes. It’s not associated with wisdom. It’s not necessarily associated with truth or with courage or with many virtues that are being likeable. Right. It’s not related at all with being likeable. Or honest. Yes, that’s right. Well, we know the psychometric relationship between intelligence and conscientiousness is zero. Right. So I think we have to make that point. Yes, I agree. I agree. I’m trying to make the point about how difficult it is for people who are on the low end of the cognitive spectrum to survive in an increasingly complex, cognitively sophisticated environment. Right. Their jobs are just disappearing. Yes, absolutely. And now let’s ask the question, is there anything that could be done about that? Well, Western society has tried very hard with a number of environmentally based interventions. Early childhood education. By the way, you said the literature in organizational psychology is very clear. The literature is equally clear in educational psychology. Oh, yeah. Well, the relationship between IQ and learning is even more powerful than the relationship between IQ and job performance. That’s right, which is kind of common sense or matches our common sense. But if you put a bunch of variables into a regression equation to predict academic achievement, and you have all these school quality variables and teacher quality variables and cognitive variables of the students, and what you find is the teacher variables and the quality of the school variables together barely account for 10% of the variance. Yeah, I know, I know. It’s terrible. It’s terrible. Well, you know, and I talked to the guy who ran admissions at Harvard. I taught at Harvard for a while. His name was Dean Whitlaw, and he is a really smart guy. I really liked Dean. And, you know, he was, let’s say he was on the right side of the human race, and he was really trying to figure out how to run the admissions policy at Harvard so that it did the best for everyone concerned. And he had run an interesting series of analysis that I don’t believe he ever published. And one of them was, well, let’s say you segregate the Harvard population into the relatively low IQ kids. So maybe they only have an IQ of 130, you know, and the relatively high IQ kids who are pushing up towards to 160. So you have two competing hypotheses there. One would be that the lower IQ kids come to Harvard, this remarkable environment, and they thrive because of the high educational quality so well that they close the gap between them and the 160 kids. And that’s just completely wrong. What happens is you put both those groups there, both very, very highly selected, but some, you know, in this sort of Superman rage intellectually, what happens is the gap just gets bigger and bigger as they progress through university. And it’s a dreaded example of that Matthew principle that the economists talk about, which is, you know, to those who have, more will be given, and from those who have nothing, everything will be taken. It’s very, very, it’s no wonder people dislike this research. It’s so anti-egalitarian in its essential structure. Well, I wanted to make a second point that I said would be provocative. If you want to do something about this, you know, we tried a bunch of interventions, earnest, well-funded, long-term interventions don’t seem to work. But neuroscience has been excluded from discussions about what to do about this. And I believe that neuroscience, the progress in neuroscience research has the potential to really dramatically increase the g factor. Well, that’s an optimistic statement. So I’m sure we’re looking forward to some support for that one. It’s optimistic and traversal. And, you know, just as a thought experiment, and I can tell you why I believe this is possible. Well, first, before I tell you the thought experiment, the reason I’m optimistic is that it is because of the high heritability of the g factor. That means, you know, if genes are involved, genes work through biology, even if environment interacts with that. But basically, you have a neurobiological system. It’s complex. But as you begin to understand it, you can tweak it. This is what all medicine is doing now. They’re trying to understand the neurobiology genetic basis. Of our health and our diseases. Why? So they can fix it. When you go to the doctor, you’re going because your biology is broken. And you want your biology fixed. Well, let’s think about the brain. Now, no one conceptualizes low IQ as a disease. And it’s a little dangerous. But to the extent to which low IQ has a genetic input or genetic influence, that’s the extent to which you might be able to find out how that works, what that system is, and then figure out how to tweak that system to increase IQ. It’s not science fiction. I mean, that’s a plausible sequence of events. The problem is it’s a very complex sequence. Sure. But I also think it’s a finite set of problems, not an infinite set of problems. If physicists can figure out what happened during the first nanoseconds of the Big Bang, we can certainly figure out what the neurobiology of intellect is and how to tweak it. I think that’s possible. So now let’s do a thought experiment. Let’s imagine there’s an IQ pill. I mean that metaphorically, not literally a pill you could take. Like having fluoridated water just kind of raises the dental health of everybody. It would be nice if we had that for IQ. But just imagine what it would be like if we shifted the distribution of IQ 15 points into the high end so that now the average IQ—and I understand how IQ is computed and norming and everything—but the point is that no one would have an IQ less than 100. Right. What would the world be like if everyone could reason sufficiently to get a reasonable job? Yeah, well that’s a good question. I mean, what would you call the perverse part of me? That’s funny. I was just talking to one of my graduate students. We’ve been looking at the determinants of male attractiveness by the personality of female viewers. Okay, and so what we found is that there are some personality effects. So extroverted, enthusiastic women tend to rate men, generally speaking, as more attractive than introverted and less enthusiastic women. So there are some just straight personality effects, but the biggest effect by far we found was the proclivity of women in general to rate men as less attractive as the women’s IQ increased. And so the other thing that we don’t know is what price we pay for accelerated IQ from a broader perspective, you know, because I know that there is some evidence—and you can tell me what you think about this—because I know that there’s reasonable evidence that the average IQ of the Ashkenazi Jewish population is about 15 points higher than the standard population, which kind of makes it a thought experiment or an experiment, real-life experiment, that’s equivalent to the one that you laid out. But Ashkenazi Jews also tend to suffer from a host of neurological diseases that seem to be associated with increased neuroplasticity. And so to me, it’s often hard to gain on one front without losing on another, you know. I mean, that’s the evolutionary conundrum, obviously. We call that the social justice theory. It’s not one thing you have to be bad on everything else to kind of balance it out. Yeah, well, generally speaking, you do pay a price for your exceptionalism, you know. That’s true because, you know, the Julian Stanley studies of the mathematically precocious kids essentially found not only were they smart, but they were more mature than their age peers, better looking, they were taller, they were physically more fit. I mean, it was kind of the anti-social. Yeah, no, no, that’s true. Well, I mean, that also might be true because one of the things that can interfere with IQ is poor health and poor nutritional quality and all of that. I mean, it doesn’t look like it’s that easy to increase IQ, but it looks like it’s pretty easy to decrease it. Yeah, you know, I think that those things have to be pretty extreme. Yeah. They have an effect and those effects may not be permanent, actually. There are some studies of deprivation of people who suffered deprivation during the Second World War that suggest that those really severe deprivations didn’t have lasting effects on them. Well, you know, well, people are pretty tough, so I’m inclined to agree with that. So have you seen any animal experimental work that you regard as compelling that shows something like the transformation of animal cognition into something that’s higher order that you regard as compelling? Not sure what you mean, but there’s certainly animal work that shows you can extract a G factor from cognitive tests given to various animals. Right. So what I was wondering is, has there been any evidence that you regard as credible showing that that could be, that that so-called animal G factor extracted in the same… We should tell our readers, too, the way you extract the G factor, I’m going to say it very, very rapidly, is imagine you take a randomly selected set of 200 questions that require abstraction of one form or another to solve. And then you give those 200 questions to 100 people and you sum the scores and you rank order them, you get something that’s roughly equivalent there to a G factor. It’s roughly that. I would say it a little bit differently, just so everyone’s on the same page. If you think of all the different mental abilities and you devise a test for each one of them, and you gave this test to a lot of people across the range of ability, what you’ll find is the scores on all those tests are positively correlated with each other, suggesting that all tests of mental ability have something in common. Right, sure, that’s a great way of putting it. Common is this G factor, this general ability to reason, and some individual tests have more G loading than other individual tests. And tests of abstract reasoning tend to be highly G loaded. If I said to you, repeat the following numbers back to me, 3, 7, 2, 1, 6, 5, that’s not a very highly G loaded mental ability to be able to do that. But if I gave you a string like that and said, repeat them to me backwards, that becomes a G loaded ability because you’re a transformation. Yes, well the other thing to say about that too is that the positive relationship between those multiple assessments that you described is actually quite high. Right. Right, that’s the thing, is that that general factor not only exists across domains of cognitive ability, but it tends to account for a substantial amount of the ability in each of those domains. It’s kind of like a, G is kind of like a black hole for intelligence research, and everything keeps falling into it. So that’s an interesting way to put it, because now we have these genome-wide association studies that are finding these bits of DNA that are related to a latent factor of intelligence, which is the G factor, or to what they call educational attainment variables. So educational attainment is so highly correlated with IQ that it’s essentially the same thing. So we’re really moving, when I was in graduate school, the question was, is there a genetic component to intelligence or not? To this kind of DNA analysis, trying to find bits of DNA that are going to be related to what we call intelligence or IQ testing or the G factor, and they seem to exist. There seems to be hundreds of them in each tiny effect, which will make the ultimate story extremely complicated. But as I said before, I think it’s a finite set of problems. Right, right. And at the end of that sequence of solving those problems, I think there’s a good chance we’ll know how to increase IQ. And I think it’s a good thing to be able to do that. You know, I’ve said publicly that more intelligence is better than less. Sometimes I get criticized because that implies that people with less intelligence aren’t as worthwhile. That’s why I want to be very careful that I don’t believe that. Yes, they have harder lives is more the accurate way of thinking about it. Harder lives in a narrower range of possibilities and opportunities. That’s right. And in my view, my political bias is therefore governments have a moral responsibility to help those people. And a lot of government programs aren’t going to do it because job training requires a certain level of G. My low IQ clients, they used to go to the government agencies that were designed to help people find employment. And the typical response was, well, just go home and type up your CV and distribute it. It’s like, I can’t use a computer. I can’t type. I don’t know what a CV is. It’s like it’s a non-starter in all three counts. That’s right. That’s right. So in the United States, there are 51 million people with IQs under 85. And there are about 43 million people living in poverty. Do you think those Venn diagrams intersect? Yeah. Well, we should also be clear about this because it is so politically suspect is that it’s not like it’s self-evident that people who have less cognitive capability are likely to end up poor because there are serious complex problems in life that beset them, that they have a difficult time dealing with, and they can’t learn as quickly. And so the relationship between poverty and intelligence is self-evident if you’re willing to think it through for any length of time. It doesn’t mean that everyone who is rich is smart. And it doesn’t mean that everyone who is poor is stupid to be blunt. But what it does mean is that if you’re intelligent, you’re much more likely to become financially successful. I think it was the Heernstein and Murray, I think, did the calculations back in the bell curve, that indicated that if you imagine that you are a fairy godmother and you have your newborn grandchild in front of you, and you can grant them three standard deviations above the mean in terms of wealth at birth, or you can grant them three standard deviations above the mean in terms of IQ at birth, and then you wanted to determine which would work better for them by the time they were 40. And the answer to that was quite clear, is that IQ trumps wealth, if it’s ability to predict a positive future. That’s why I’m so interested in the concept of increasing IQ or increasing the G factor, not just the IQ score, but really what under this reasoning ability. So some people have tried to teach college students critical thinking. I think that’s a good thing. It is. You’re smart and you can think critically so much the better. Exactly. And it may sound to your listeners, I just want to take a moment out here, it may sound to your listeners like, here are these two guys pontificating about what it’s like to be smart, what it’s like to be not so smart. I mean, the point of this, the point of neuroscience research on intelligence, and what I hope to achieve by writing the book, was to show that the genetic aspects are not deterministic. It’s the opposite. It’s probabilistic. So the extent to which something like intelligence is genetic, in my view, is the extent to which we’ll learn how to change it for the better. Right. Well, that’s definitely, yeah, because people do tend to think about biological factors as deterministic, and that’s a mistake because they can be shifted. So with regards to the animal study, so you pointed out that you can come up with an IQ-like estimate for, say, a rat. And have you seen anything that indicates, I don’t care how it’s done, through training or neurochemically or by promoting brain function in different ways, however you might do it, that’s actually indicated to you that there is a way of biologically enhancing the general cognitive ability even of an animal? Has anything come out credible? Just by breeding mice who run a maze faster than other mice, together you get mice that seem to be able to learn how to run a maze faster. But that’s work from the 1980s. I review this in the book. There are some interesting technologies that have been developed where you can turn parts of a rat brain on and off at will and see what happens. This has not yet been applied, as far as I know, to learning. But interestingly, there are human studies underway with things like trans-cranial magnetic stimulation and low voltage ways to stimulate parts of the brain. There are some interesting experiments now being done with humans to see if you can improve learning or reasoning ability. As a matter of fact, I edit this journal called Intelligence, which is a prime spot for intelligence researchers to publish on all aspects of intelligence. We’re just starting to put together a special issue on human experiments to increase reasoning ability using these techniques of stimulating the brain. Have you seen some positive things with regards to low level electrical stimulation? It’s more anecdotal, but there’s a bit of research being used to treat depression, for example. That’s right. I do cover in the book the studies that were published up until the time I wrote the book and cautioned people that they hadn’t been replicated yet. Right. We’re on the way. This is an evolving area that’s going to be very exciting. If students are listening to this podcast and they’re thinking about neuroscience or psychology, this kind of experiment is really a new phase of intelligence research where you can do experiments on human beings that are completely ethical and relatively non-intrusive. This is really going to change everything now because it will shift intelligence research from basically psychometric correlations past what neuroimaging has done, which really moved it away from just psychometrics. Then correlated psychometric scores with measurable aspects of the brain like glucose metabolic function or the amount of gray matter or white matter or the number of white matter fibers. There are all these fabulous connections. Right. It was a new phase. I got in early on that phase, but that phase is now moving into this new phase of actually stimulating the brain to improve learning and memory and reasoning and all the while doing it with neuroimaging. Right. See what happens and adding DNA to it. I mean, come on now. This is a great time to be entering the research in this area. Right. Well, there’s some optimism on the horizon. I mean, I looked for a while because I’ve been very interested in improving human performance, measuring it and improving it. And so I look and I’m doing that also in conjunction with businesses because I like things to have a practical end. And I looked to find out what the research indicated with regards to improvement of intelligence. And mostly what I found was not so much improvement as conservation is that if you exercise both aerobically and with weight lifting, that that can help you maintain your fluid intelligence for longer across your lifespan because it tends to decline rather precipitously as you age, which is one of the more dismal things that you also discover with IQ research and starts to decline when you’re in your early 20s. And it’s kind of linear downhill all the way along. But exercise really helps. But it’s preservation, which is pretty important at my age. Yeah. Preservation is a big deal, man, but enhancement would be good. But you know, this thing about an IQ pill coming up with a way to manipulate the neurobiology of your brain regarding intellect. If there’s a breakthrough in this, it will come either from Alzheimer’s research or formal aging research, trying to prevent the slow decline of your mental faculties as you age, especially fluid intelligence, or trying to reverse the ravages of Alzheimer’s disease. These are neurochemical problems. Right. Well, an IQ seems to be quite tightly linked to. Well, this is another thing we could talk about because I know that there are certain biological markers that IQ is loosely associated with. So it predicts in a fragmentary matter. So I know that even something as simple as simple reaction time, how fast you can push a button when a light comes on, is correlated with IQ, with fluid intelligence at about point two. And having a bigger head is slightly correlated, especially when you correct for body size. And so is brain mass. And so is thickness of the myelin sheaths on individual neurons. And so there are these micro markers of, you might think about them as neurological integrity that seem to predict IQ. But you’ve been doing neuroimaging, and I’m not as up on that. I haven’t looked at that for a couple of years. So what have the neuroimagers found about brain structure and function in relationship to intelligence that you think is compelling and interesting? Glad you asked this because as I was finishing the manuscript for the book, literally the day after I turned it in, I had to ask for it back because there was this very interesting study published by a group at Yale that used a fairly sophisticated way to look at white matter connections, structural white matter connections and functional connections in the brain. And determining how one brain area is functionally or structurally related to all other brain areas. And you can put up a map of a person’s brain that shows from brain imaging, from MRI technology, how their brain is interconnected. And this paper said these interconnections are so reliable within a person that they’re like fingerprints. And not only that, but the fingerprints can predict IQ. And so is it density of interconnections or something like that or is there something more specific going on? It can be the density of connections structurally, how much white matter connects this area to that area. And there are certain brain areas where you have a lot of white matter coming in and a lot of white matter going out to other parts of the brain. They’re called hubs and there are nodes that have lesser connections. And so it makes kind of, it certainly makes sense that being able to make measurements of brain connectivity would be related to things like intelligence. Do you remember what some of the major hubs were? Like are they identifiable also as neuroanatomical areas with specific functions? They’re definitely neuroanatomical areas. And they’re what you might expect, but what was exciting to me is they mapped onto a model of brain intelligence relationships that I had developed with my colleague, Rex Jung and published in 2007. And it’s called the parietal frontal integration theory or PFIT of intelligence. And the idea is that the connections between the parietal world, which is here and the frontal area are the key connections for intelligence. Okay. So tell us, well, tell us why you, you derived that particular theory, because, you know, people have suggested, say, alternatively that the seat of higher order intelligence is basically, let’s say the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex or something like that. So why specifically the connection patterns between frontal and parietal areas? Well, this article in 2007 was a review article where we took every single brain imaging study we could find that included a measure of intelligence. And there were 37 such studies at the time, including some I had done as early as 1988 and others had done with much larger samples. And we just kind of qualitatively analyzed the results to see what brain areas came up in common across these studies using different measures, different imaging techniques. And we found that there was not an, that not all brain areas were equally distributed. They tended to be concentrated in the front and the parietal lobe, but also we found areas in the occipital lobe and the temporal lobe that were also related to intelligence. And so we developed this model that we talked about how information would be processed and how information would flow around this set of, I think there were 18 areas all and the hypothesize that sport high on intelligence tests would have some combination of these areas. You didn’t need all of them kind of working together, but some people would have this combination. Some people would have that combination. And if you could make measurements about the way information was flowing around these areas with a technology like the magnetoencephalogram, which shows changes in the brain millisecond by millisecond, then you might be able to actually estimate IQ from brain images. 2007, people were trying to do this with multiple regression equations that never really replicated. Independent replications didn’t go very far because the sample sizes were relatively small. You had enormous individual differences, but these newer techniques, these mathematical techniques of calculating brain connectivity really seemed to have advanced this whole thing dramatically. Was there a map between the nodes that were identified in this more recent research in the areas that you guys had identified with your overarching analysis? Yes, the way Rex Jung and I looked at the data, it seemed like there was considerable overlap, and some of the authors who we did not know personally when they wrote their papers noted that their findings were consistent with our model. Any hemispheric differences? Yes, there were more on the left than on the right, but there were also areas on the right as well. And these areas tend to be areas that are also related to language and memory and attention. So the more fundamental cognitive processes of language and memory and attention seem to be the architecture on which intelligence is built. Here’s a question that I haven’t been able to figure out because I’ve looked at the attention literature a lot, and the more I look at the attention literature, the more I find it difficult to distinguish it from the intelligence literature. I mean, attention and intelligence seem to be different things, and we certainly use the words in common parlance as different, but I haven’t really been able to… Imagine you wanted to establish a battery of attention-related tests that were independent of G-loaded cognitive abilities. I haven’t seen anybody manage that. And so what do you think the difference is between the capacity to pay attention, which also seems to be associated with conscientiousness, by the way, which isn’t associated with IQ. But I mean, what’s the relationship between attention and intelligence as far as you’re concerned? Those studies have been done where they take cognitive variables, the elemental cognitive tasks is what they call them, the real basic things that cognitive psychologists like to study, because they like to study reasoning. Right. And they all study learning and memory, but they don’t want to address why some people learn faster than other people, or why some people can remember more than other people. That’s what cognitive psychologists study. They study what’s common to everybody. But if you look at these elemental cognitive tasks, you can extract a G factor of cognition, which is highly correlated with a psychometric G factor abilities. More than attention, memory, aspects of memory are more correlated to the G factor. Processing speed is correlated. And attention is also correlated. So I kind of have this idea from being a parent, watching my kids grow up, that people differ in their baseline of attention when they’re not specifically paying attention. This might be called consciousness. So you have two kids walking through a museum for an hour, and you come out and you say to kid one, so what did you see? And you get a whole long thing. And you ask the other kid, well, what did you see there? And you get a much less rich explanation. Yeah. I’ve always thought about that as a difference in resolution of worlds. Well, call it what you will, but there are these differences. We’ve actually studied consciousness with brain imaging with my friend Mike Alkyer, who’s an anesthesiologist. We did the first imaging studies where we brought normal volunteers and Mike gave them anesthetic drugs to knock them out completely while they underwent brain imaging. And we had different levels of anesthesia. We were trying to see what part of the brain is the last part of the brain to turn off when you lose consciousness. And did you find anything that you could make sense out of from that? Because of course, that’s obviously an extraordinarily interesting question. I mean, is it the collapse of these networks? Mike Alkyer is the real expert on this. He’s published a whole series of papers. And the mechanism of consciousness is still one of the great Nobel Prize winning. Yeah. Well, that’s my interpretation of literature too. It’s such a mystery that it seems uncrackable in some sense. What was good about the imaging stuff here, you could study this experimentally. You could put people into different levels of consciousness and bring them out at will and see how the brain reacted. So the thalamus seems to be important. A lot of people are looking at thalamus connections. And each anesthetic, there are categories of anesthetics. And they have different mechanisms of action in the brain. Yet they all produce the same consequence when you lose consciousness. Right. That’s a very interesting thing too. I know with the thalamic, what is it? Cortical thalamic loops. I mean, there’s a guy named, what’s his name? Voldemire? God, it’s close to that. I’m afraid I haven’t got it right. He suggests that one of the consequences of psychedelic drugs is to decrease the gating of the thalamic cortical circuitry. And that’s one of the mechanisms whereby that expanded at least sense of consciousness emerges as a consequence of experimentation with the psychedelic end of the pharmaceutical universe. Yeah. I have a section in the book where I talk about consciousness and these studies and try to relate the concept that if you can turn consciousness off, you should be able to turn it on. We know anesthesiologists do this at will. Even though they don’t understand what they’re doing. Right, right. They can’t tell you why it works. But then can you use, if anesthetic drugs kind of dissociate the brain and creativity seems to be related to a dissociation of the frontal logs, can low doses, very, very low doses of anesthetic drugs cause just enough disinhibition to increase your creativity? And? I don’t think this experiment has ever been done. You see something similar in that sometimes reported with people who develop frontotemporal dementia. That’s exactly right. You know, is that as, and that’s a very strange phenomenon where as your brain deteriorates, your creativity increases because so much of brain function seems to be inhibitory. That’s right. I don’t know of a comparable disease that produces increases in intelligence. Right. No, no, I’ve never heard of that. Oh yeah, the other thing that’s been, so you can tell me what you think about this. I mean, I was curious for a while about these companies like Lumosity because when we developed our original prefrontal tasks, they were tests, they were, they kind of had a game like element, you know, and we kind of thought, well, maybe if you had people practice doing them, they would obviously get better at the specific task because that is what people do. But then if you had people practice a whole bunch of them, maybe they would get better at the entirety of the tasks in a way that would generalize to other measures like the Raven’s progressive matrices, which is a good measure of fluid intelligence, but that never works. And of course, Lumosity claimed that they were able to produce enhancement in general intelligence, but by all appearances, that’s been a dismal failure as well. It’s very strange in some sense that that general factor doesn’t seem to be something that you can actually improve by practice. Right. Well, you know, what the hell? Why? Why? I can’t just, I just don’t get that. It doesn’t make sense, but here’s something you don’t know about me, Albert, but I am in the Guinness book of records, the gamers edition because of my study, my brain imaging study of Tetris. And I did a brain imaging study of Tetris. I think it was published in 1992. I really wanted to do a study of warning. I wanted to see what happened in the brain before and after you learned something. And back in 1991, when I was doing this, nobody had personal computers. Nobody had ever heard of Tetris. Computer games were not what they are today. And I went to the egghead software store when they had software stores that had just opened. And I was talking to the guys there about I needed something where I could study before and after they warned something relatively simple. They showed me this game Tetris. It had just come in. They just had opened the box. They put it up on the computer there. And I thought, boy, this is really perfect. It’s simple to learn, but there’s an enormous learning curve. And we brought in college students, a small number of college students, and we showed them how to play Tetris. They practiced for a few minutes. We injected the radioactive glucose for positron emission tomography. And we did PET scan studies of them that very first day they played Tetris for 32 minutes. They then practiced every day. They had to come to my office because nobody had a PC at home. They came to my office five days a week for about four weeks. They practiced until they got so good at Tetris. The game was moving faster. Yeah. Yeah. It’s amazing how good people can get at that. Yeah. You couldn’t even believe a human being could do this. Yeah. I know. I’ve watched people do that sort of thing. It’s just absolutely unbelievable how good they can get at it. Right. And when they got really good, we scanned them a second time. And we found that even though the game was faster and harder, when they had learned how to do it, they used less glucose metabolic rate. Okay. So I knew that research. I read that research. Now, what do you think about the… Now, the problem is I can’t remember where this research came from because I also read at approximately the same time studies that appeared to claim, and I think this was reviewed by Alcona Goldberg in his book on hemispheric specialization for routinization and novelty, respectively, that as you… When you first start to learn something novel, and I think this was demonstrated, for example, in people who were listening… They were Danish native speakers who were listening to Danish in reverse, and they used very large portions of their brain when they were listening to Danish in reverse. But if they were listening to Danish property spoken, they used very small specified parts of the brain that were located in the back part of the left hemisphere. And there was another group of researchers who were demonstrating that as you learned, the degree of activation decreased, and it shifted from the right to the left, and it shifted from the front to the back, and it got smaller and smaller. And is that associated with that decreased glucose utilization? Is that the same phenomenon? I think it is. I think we were the first to show it. We had done one imaging study before this Tetris study where we just correlated glucose metabolic rate with scores on a test of abstract reasoning, a high G-loaded test. They had taken the test during the imaging, so we got to see what brain areas were involved. And we did find some brain areas, but the really interesting, surprising thing was the correlation between the scores and glucose metabolic rate was always negative. The better they did on the test, the lower their glucose metabolic rate. And that was the first inkling we had about this idea of brain efficiency. There’s been a lot of research on that. It turns out to be a complicated thing because nothing about the brain is simple. But it does seem to make perfect sense that expert skill is associated with doing more with less. It makes sense now, but I tell you, nobody predicted it. No, no. Look, I understand. That research stood out for me in a very striking manner. But it also gets at this thing that you raised about practicing on different kinds of tests. Because one of the attractive things about Tetris is it’s visual spatial, it’s planning ahead, or it’s attention. There are a lot of elemental cognitive tasks necessary. It’s fine motor control, really necessary to do well on Tetris and to learn it really well. And so the Tetris company found my research some years later and asked if I’d be willing to try to replicate it with more modern imaging, which of course I was willing to do. So they funded this. Now instead of PET scanning, we use functional MRI and structural MRI, both. And we did find, like we found with the PET scanning, there were areas where after teenage girls with very limited gaming experience were in Tetris. Their brain activity decreased. But we also found from the structural MRI that there were increases in gray matter. And the really interesting thing is the areas where there were increases in gray matter did not overlap at all with the areas that functionally decreased. It would have been a terrific story. Wow, that’s strange. Yeah, well things are always more complicated than you hope them to be. Not only that, but I can tell you that every time I did a brain imaging study, we always found the exact opposite of what we expected. That sounds to me like you might actually be operating as a real scientist. I mean things are so damn complicated that it’s really difficult to guess right to begin with. Well it led to one of my three laws that I based the book on. Law number one is no story about the brain is simple. Law number two is no one study is definitive. And law number three is it takes a long time to sort out all the various studies to see what’s consistent and what establishes a reliable weight of evidence. So let me put you on a different track momentarily. And maybe this won’t work, but you know I’m always curious about let’s say the practical implications of scientific research both at a personal level, familial level, social level, all of those things. You know so I mean one of the things that I’m planning to do in the near future is to launch a website that will enable people to assess themselves with what we’ve developed a scale called the big five aspect scale that breaks the big five down into ten aspects each of which which provide some additional high resolution and useful descriptions of personality. So you’ll be able to go there and find out what your personality is like. You’ll be able to compare yourself to other people who you know to find out where your similarities and differences are. But one of the things we’ve been thinking about doing as well is putting up on the same site a real IQ test like nicely validated probably focusing on fluid intelligence because it’s a little bit less linguistically complex to do so but maybe measuring verbal intelligence and then showing people the strata of occupations in which they’re likely to find maximal success. Because you know from for me given that I know that people vary in their cognitive abilities tremendously and that that’s actually an important determinant of their life outcome it seems to me so let’s say someone tests out in an IQ of around 115 and so you can say look you know you’re pretty damn smart you’re you’re up above 85 percent of the general population you you could you could probably do a pretty damn good job as an undergraduate in university if you were also disciplined right if you were conscientious you hit the books hard you’re gonna you’re gonna come out in the top quartile of your class assuming that you’re not at a spectacularly success spectacularly selective university but you’re gonna have a much more difficult time as a master student and PhD level stuff is going to be you’re going to be pushing your luck to to really master that but you know you could be here’s a here’s a domain of of of of of what would you call it industrial organizational activity where you could really be in the top 10 percentile you know it’s like so if you have an IQ of 115 like you might make one bang up plumber and you can have a spectacularly successful career as a plumber and maybe as a manager of other plumbers and all of that and you know and I mean I actually happen to be a real aficionado of the trade so I certainly don’t think of that as something that’s a that’s a low quality or low status occupation in the least but we would we would like to tell people okay here’s here’s here’s a here’s an intellectual domain that’s probably too high for you to be successful without working insane hours to to to close the gap because you can do that with insane work up to some some limited degree but it seems to me that the logical thing to do at least in part is to give people a sense of what their advantages and limitations are and then say to them okay well given that here is a place that you could go where you could be optimally successful and so you know that’s kind of my take from a policy perspective let’s say but like what have you thought about you know the massive diversity and intellectual ability I mean what are the implications from a policy perspective as far as you’re concerned well I think vocational guidance is clearly one I actually did some consulting and some research for a non-profit group called the Johnson O’Connor Foundation which is vocational testing they’re not that big on g but they have their own battery of tests they bring someone in and do a full day of cognitive tests and from that they give some advice about what kinds of professions match their cognitive strengths and weaknesses and I actually did some brain imaging on their tests and was very you know very interesting stuff did you think they do did you think see because it’s hard to do the psychometrics properly with regards to vocational guidance because we don’t really know we don’t have a good handle on how to classify jobs into their various subtypes like John Holland has done some good work doing that but but there’s so many jobs and it’s hard to figure out well what makes two jobs the same or similar you know no I was there Hopkins when Holland was there and some of my friends were graduate students worked for him and I learned all about that vocational testing it’s very powerful and as you know it’s that’s more his his scales have kind of morphed into more personality like dimensions yeah well that’s it you want that well that’s exactly the nexus that we want to play out it’s like okay because there is a reason I know people have been mapping Holland’s Holland’s job categories onto the big five and with a fair bit of success you know and we’re hoping that the differentiation down to the to 10 levels of personality will provide even more precision but but with more general policy like okay so fine so reasonable vocational counseling that’s a good idea when does it start does start in junior high like do you do what the Europeans do and start to track people into trades and and and higher education at that kind of early age the Europeans seem to have had great success with that so it’s Germans in particular yeah so it’s reasonable to look at but the problem in the United States there are so many problems with the way we conceptualize education and the whole idea of tracking I don’t know how it is in Canada but in the United States this idea of tracking has a very negative balance to it you know segregating the smart kids into one one set of classes and the less smart kids into others and then there’s media education and there’s been a tendency a strong tendency in the United States to feel that kids learn from each other so you don’t want homogeneous groups based on learning ability you want yeah it’s so funny though because it’s so funny because the people say that with regards to to let’s say academic achievement but they play exactly the opposite stunt when it comes to such things as as childhood sports you know so if you look at football for example it’s like well hey let’s segregate like ability it’s like you don’t have the people who stumble around on the field dropping the ball all the time playing with the top end quarterbacks and nobody thinks there’s a problem with that they don’t say well everyone learns from everyone situation so to me it speaks more of a refusal to admit to the stark reality that there are massive cognitive differences between people and to try to actually start to address that with some degree of seriousness and the seriousness should be something like okay well let’s look at the bottom 15 percent of the population cognitively speaking it’s like what the hell can we do for those people that’s going to be useful and you know like the guy that I was telling you about I was trying to think of some way that he could find a respectable and productive and relatively stable position in society that would be useful and I thought there was a couple of things he could do like one of the things he could have done I think he could have been encouraged let’s say to to collect trash in the downtown areas like he could have been assigned a city block and and it could have been said to him look your job is to keep this damn city block clean here’s a bag here’s a stick you get up in the morning you go do this it’s like makes everybody’s life more pleasant it’s a valuable contribution it’s something you could do with a certain degree of pride and it and there’s a socially valuable end of it now what happens in Toronto is that people drive around these vacuum cleaner machines on sidewalks and pick up the scrap paper and all of that that actually turns out to be a very cognitively demanding job because well you have to pay careful attention you can’t run over people you have to have decent social skills and you know it’s complicated but it might be nice to see but we but we’re not mature enough to have a discussion like this as a society we might want to say it’s like okay well there’s a group of people who aren’t going to be able to compete in the in the cognitive workplace they’re not going to do it and there’s actually lots of them and we’re not going to say they’re lazy and we’re not going to say they’re not looking for work we’re not going to say any of that we’re going to say look we need to find occupations that have public utility that aren’t just make work projects that people of that level of abstract capacity could actually perform but I don’t think we have the maturity to have that conversation we don’t have the economics and so I would go even a little to the left of you and I would say there’s nothing wrong with make work programs you know to to allow people I mean you have you know to to work with dignity so you have that option but you also have this interesting experiment being proposed of the minimum annual income yeah well that one worries me because see there’s a couple of things about that one that concerns me because you know so we’re having a conversation here where we’re taking differences in iq seriously but the problem with the guaranteed annual income issue I think one of the problems and I’m not denying its potential utility it’s something I think that would have to be experimentally determined you know and all of that and maybe it could replace a plethora of less efficient social welfare programs but you know it isn’t obvious to me it’s obvious to me that there’s a substantial proportion of the population and I would say it’s probably five percent that would destroy themselves instantly if you gave them a guaranteed annual income and they would do it because they’re very low in conscientiousness for example and very impulsive and like I’ve worked with many guys often ex-calcane addicts who were often not all that high on the end of the cognitive distribution but very very low in conscientiousness high in impulsivity and those guys were absolutely fine as long as they were flat broke but man I tell you as soon as they had money they were done it was like three days in the bar cocaine binge face down in a ditch and then they were fine until they got money again and so I don’t the problem with the guaranteed annual income solution is that you know man does not live by bread alone let’s say and that if you have money and you have things to do then you have a life but if you just have money you don’t have a life well you know I look at you know I have a very narrow lens on this what do I know about the big social policies and you know I don’t know anything about that more than anybody else I have opinions like everybody but my narrow lens is through intelligence yeah I think more is better than less yeah more doesn’t make you a better person doesn’t make you honest doesn’t make you likeable doesn’t make you conscientious it doesn’t solve the problems of the world but if we can do something to increase intelligence generally I think that would go in some measure toward alleviating a lot of these these very complex problems so I mean imagine the homelessness problem some proportion of chronically homeless people have schizophrenia yeah schizophrenia is a genetic disorder nobody knows what the genes are or how they work but it’s pretty clear there’s a genetic component if you can find the genes figure out what they do and come up with real good treatments if not cures for schizophrenia and possibly preventions for schizophrenia that indirectly is going to help alleviate the homeless problem right so I don’t know what to do about the homeless but if you can figure out what to do about schizophrenia that’s going to have some impact on that yes it’s basically yeah well use you know your claim to some degree is that these more complex social problems should be decomposed into isolatable micro problems and that specific solution should be sought for them and that that certainly strikes me as an appropriate approach because everybody is different not everyone’s home for the same reason no no well that’s for sure that’s for sure not everyone’s poor for the same for the same reason by any stretch of the imagination right that’s right poverty is doesn’t have one cause no but the piece and it’s not merely caused by lack of money either because that would be a much easier problem to solve unfortunately it’s not so simple so the one thing that we’re probably getting toward the end the one thing I would like to leave your listeners with is really optimism these are not dismal problems in perpetuity because the genetic approach I think and I’m kind of out you know I’m kind of out there on this this is not a mainstream view but I think the more something is genetic the more likely it is we can change it for the good and that’s why we should study the genetics of intelligence that’s why I hope genes have more to do with it that environment not you know not yet because I want to do something nefarious with genes and I understand the history of this very yes yes because I think in neuroscience there’s there’s optimism and to guard against the negative aspects the potential negative aspects of this the only solution is to have public conversation about this and to get people to understand iq intelligence g factor these are not all the same things they have different relationships to the data to the genetic data particularly so we have to understand these things and once we have some sense of this and not hate people who believe in that there’s a genetic component as as a priori racist or malevolent people I think we we can come to a common understanding about how genes and environment interact and figure out how all that works and begin to think how in the year 2050 or 2060 we’re going to have to resolve people have resolved some of these issues to practical benefit well that is a good place to leave it I think and we’ve had a very productive discussion so I think that’s probably a nice place to to to bring everything to a close I’ve been speaking with dr. Richard higher who studies the neural basis of human intelligence and cognition and who has recently released a new book called the neuroscience of intelligence published by Cambridge University Press it’s an academic book but for people who are interested in a serious discussion of the relationship between biology and intelligence then it’s a good go-to tome and it’s it’s an extraordinarily important topic given the undeniable primacy of intelligence as the fundamental predictor of of of let’s say success in many many domains in across the human lifespan so thank you very much for spending an hour or so with us today and uh and with any luck maybe we’ll talk to you again in the future I’d like to talk to you perhaps about consciousness at some point that might be entertaining well listen I’ve really enjoyed it I appreciate the chance of having more than three sentences you know three sentences you know related to to your listeners I just want to say one thing about the book it is written for the lay public as well okay well that’s a good thing to know Cambridge wouldn’t let me call it the neuroscience of intelligence calling what every parent and student needs to know about neuroscience but because they wanted to market it as a strictly academic book but it’s getting quite a bit of attention just among non-academic readers okay well excellent well I’ll make sure that I I mentioned that in the description which I will I’ll post a link to the book probably on Amazon I that’s usually the most straightforward thing and also point out that it is in fact written for people who aren’t only specialists in the area so that was good thanks yep thanks a lot hey listen thank you very much I appreciate it no problem it’s a pleasure talking with you too good to meet you okay okay bye bye